Table of Contents

Introductions to R

Short Course

1st SMEP Short Course

2nd SMEP Short Course

ARP Short Course

Documentation for the psych package

An introduction to psychometric theory with applications in R

An Overview

This page is devoted to teaching others about psychometric theory as well as R. It consists of chapters of an in progress text as well as various short courses on R.

The e-book is a work in progress. Chapters will appear sporadically. Parts of it are from the draft of a book being prepared for the Springer series on using R, other parts are just interesting tid-bits that would not be appropriate as chapters.

It is written in the hope that I can instill in a new generation of psychologists the love for quantitative methodology imparted to me by reading the popular and then later the scientific texts of Ray Cattell [Cattell, 1966b] and Hans Eysenck [Eysenck, 1964, Eysenck, 1953, Eysenck, 1965]. Those Penguin and Pelican paperbacks by Cattell and Eysenck were the first indications that I had that it was possible to study personality and psychology with a quantitative approach.

My course in psychometric theory, on which much of this book is based, was inspired by a course of the same name by Warren Norman. The organizational structure of this text owes a great deal to the structure of Warren's course. Warren introduced me, as well as a generation of graduate students at the University of Michigan, to the role of theory and measurement in the study of psychology. He introduced to me to the "bible" of psychometrics: Jum Nunnally's Psychometric Theory [Nunnally, 1967].

The students in my psychometric theory classes over the years, by their continuing questions and sometimes confusion, have given me the motivation to try to make this text as understandable and useful as I can. The members of the Society of Multivariate Experimental Psychology, by their willingness to share cutting (and sometimes bleeding) edge ideas freely and with respect for alternative interpretations have been a never ending source of new and exciting ideas.

This book would not be possible without the amazing contributions of the R-Core Team and the many contributers to R and the R-Help listserve.

Chapter 1: Introduction - PDF

Psychometrics is that area of psychology that specializes in how to measure what we talk and think about. It is how to assign numbers to observations in a way that best allows us to summarize our observations in order to advance our knowledge. Although in particular it is the study of how to measure psychological constructs, the techniques of psychometrics are applicable to most problems in measurement. The measurement of intelligence, extraversion, severity of crimes, or even batting averages in baseball are all grist for the psychometric mill. Any set of observations that are not perfect exemplars of the construct of interest is open to questions of reliability and validity and to psychometric analysis.

Although it is possible to make the study of psychometrics seem dauntingly difficult, in fact the basic concepts are straightforward. This text is an attempt to introduce the fundamental concepts in psychometric theory so that the reader will be able to understand how to apply them to real data sets of interest. It is not meant to make one an expert, but merely to instill confidence and an understanding of the fundamentals of measurement so that the reader can better understand and contribute to the research enterprise.

Chapter 2: A Theory of Data - PDF

At first glance, it would seem that we have an infinite way to collect data. Measuring the diameter of the earth by finding the distance to the horizon, measuring the height of waves produced by a nuclear blast by nailing (empty) beer cans to a palm tree, or finding Avogadro's number by dropping oil into water are techniques that do not require great sophistication in the theory of measurement. In psychology we can use self report, peer ratings, reaction times, psychophysiological measures such as the Electric Encephelagram (EEG), the basal level of Skin Conductance (SC), or the Galvanic Skin Response (GSR). We can measure the number of voxels showing activation greater than some threshold in a functional Magnetic Resonance Image (fMRI), or we can measure life time risk of cancer, length of life, risk of mortality, etc. Indeed, the basic forms of data we can collect probably are unlimited. But in fact, it is possible to organize these disparate forms of data in terms of an abstract organization in terms of what is being measured and in comparison to what.

Chapter 3: The Problem of Scale - PDF

The challenge of psychometrics is assign numbers to observations in a way that best summarizes the underlying constructs. The ways to collect observations are multiple and can be based upon comparisons of order or of proximity (Chapter 2). But given a set of observations, how best to describe them? This is a problem not just for observational but also for experimental psychologists for both approaches are attempting to make inferences about latent variables in terms of statistics based upon observed variables.

For the experimentalist, the problem becomes interpreting the effect of an experimental manipulation upon some outcome variable in terms of the effect of manipulation on the latent outcome variable and the relationship between the latent and observed outcome variables. For the observationalist, the observed correlation between the observed Person Variable and Outcome variable is interpreted as a function of the relationship between the latent person trait variable and the observed trait variable, the latent outcome variable and the observed outcome variable and most importantly for inference, the relationship between the two latent variables.

Chapter 4: Correlation and Covariance - PDF

A fundamental question in science is how to measure the relationship between two variables. The answer, developed in the late 19th century, in the the form of the correlation coefficient is arguably the most important contribution to psychological theory and method- ology in the past two centuries. Whether we are examining the effect of education upon later income, of parental height upon the height of offspring, or the likelihood of graduating from college as a function of SAT score, the question remains the same: what is the strength of the relationship? This chapter examines measures of relationship between two variables. Generalizations to the problem of how to measure the relationships between sets of variables (multiple correlation and multiple regression) are left to Chapter 5.

Chapter 5: Multiple Correlation and Multiple Regression - PDF

The previous chapter considered how to determine the relationship between two variables and how to predict one from the other. The general solution was to consider the ratio of the covariance between two variables to the variance of the predictor variable (regression) or the ratio of the covariance to the square root of the product the variances (correlation). This solution may be generalized to the problem of how to predict a single variable from the weighted linear sum of multiple variables (multiple regression) or to measure the strength of this relationship (multiple correlation). As part of the problem of finding the weights, the concepts of partial covariance and partial correlation will be introduced. To do all of this will require finding the variance of a composite score, and the covariance of this composite with another score, which might itself be a composite. Much of psychometric theory is merely an extension, an elaboration, or a generalization of these concepts. Almost all tests are composites of items or subtests. An understanding how to decompose test variance into its component parts, and conversely, an understanding how to analyze tests as composites of items, allows us to analyze the meaning of tests. But tests are not merely composites of items. Tests relate to other tests. A deep appreciation of the basic Pearson correlation coefficient facilitates an understanding of its generalization to multiple and partial correlation, to factor analysis, and to questions of validity.

Chapter 6: Constructs, Components, and Factor Models - PDF

Parsimony of description has been a goal of science since at least the famous dictum commonly attributed to William of Ockham to not multiply entities beyond necessity1. The goal for parsimony is seen in psychometrics as an attempt either to describe (components) or to explain (factors) the relationships between many observed variables in terms of a more limited set of components or latent factors.

The typical data matrix represents multiple items or scales usually thought to reflect fewer underlying constructs2. At the most simple, a set of items can be be thought of representing random samples from one underlying domain or perhaps a small set of domains. The ques- tion for the psychometrician is how many domains are represented and how well does each item represent the domains. Solutions to this problem are examples of factor analysis (FA), principal components analysis (PCA), and cluster analysis (CA). All of these procedures aim to reduce the complexity of the observed data. In the case of FA, the goal is to identify fewer underlying constructs to explain the observed data. In the case of PCA, the goal can be mere data reduction, but the interpretation of components is frequently done in terms similar to those used when describing the latent variables estimated by FA. Cluster analytic techniques, although usually used to partition the subject space rather than the variable space, can also be used to group variables to reduce the complexity of the data by forming fewer and more homogeneous sets of tests or items.

Chapter 7: Reliability - PDF

Whether discussing ability, affect, or climate change, as scientists we are interested in the relationships between our theoretical constructs. We recognize, however, that our measure- ments are not perfect and that any particular observation has some unknown amount of error associated with that measurement for “all measurement is befuddled by error (McNemar, 1946, p 294). When estimating central tendencies, the confidence interval of the mean may be estimated by the standard error of the sample observations, and may be calculated from the observed standard deviation and the number of observations. This is, of course, the basic concept of Gossett and Fisher.

Error may be both random as well systematic. Random error reflects trial by trial variabil- ity due to unknown sources while systematic error may reflect situational or individual effects that may be specified. Perhaps the classic example of systematic error (known as the personal equation) is the analysis of individual differences in reaction time in making astronomical ob- servations. "The personal equation of an observer is the interval of time which habitually intervenes between the actual and the observed transit of a star..." (Rogers, 1869). Before systematic individual differences were analyzed, the British astronomer Maskelyn fired his as- sistant Kennebrook for making measurements that did not agree with his own (Stigler, 1986). Subsequent investigations of the systematic bias (Safford, 1898; Sanford, 1889) showed con- sistent individual differences as well as the effect of situational manipulations such as hunger and sleep deprivation (Rogers, 1869).

Chapter 8: Item Response Theory - PDF

Classical test theory is concerned with the reliability of a test and assumes that the items within the test are sampled at random from a domain of relevant items. Reliability is seen as a characteristic of the test and of the variance of the trait it measures. Items are treated as random replicates of each other and their characteristics, if examined at all, are expressed as correlations with total test score or as factor loadings on the putative latent variable(s) of interest. Characteristics of their properties are not analyzed in detail. This led Mellenbergh (1996) to the distinction between theories of tests (Lord and Novick, 1968) and a theories of items (Lord, 1952; Rasch, 1960). The so-called "New Psychometrics" (Embretson and Hershberger, 1999; Embretson and Reise, 2000; Van der Linden and Hambleton, 1997) is a theory of how people respond to items and is known as Item Response Theory or IRT. Over the past twenty years there has been explosive growth in programs that can do IRT, and within R there are at least four very powerful packages: eRm (Mair and Hatzinger, 2007), ltm Rizopoulos (2006), lme4 (Doran et al., 2007) and MiscPsycho, (Doran, 2010). Additional packages include mokken (van der Ark, 2010) to do non-metric IRT and plink (Weeks, 2010) to link multiple groups together. More IRT packages are being added all of the time.

Appendix A: Getting Started (with R)

Appendix B: R Commands

Appendix E: Review of Matrix Algebra

References