Evolutionary Theory and Behavior Genetics
- 1. Evolutionary theory and species typical behavior
- a) why individual differences?
- (1) if something is important to survival, it should be selected for and there should not be within species variation
- (2) balanced polymorphisms
- (a) the example of sickle cell
- (b) adaptive advantage of mid level of personality
- i) approach traits
- ii) avoidance traits
b) 5 types of competition
- (1) between species
- (a) adaptation rates
- (b) sexual reproduction as a response to pathogens and parasites
- (2) within species
- (a) intra-sexual competition for survival and reproduction
- i) niche selection
- ii) multiple strategies
- (b) inter-sexual competition for resources
- i) maternity certainty and high investment
- ii) paternity uncertainty and lower investment
- (c) parent-offspring competition
- (d) sibling competition
- i) differential reproductive fitness as a function of birth order
- ii) multiple strategies in response to birth order differences
2. Behavior genetics and the inheritance of individual differences
- a) Behavior genetics: experiments of nature
- (1) structural genes
- (2) regulatory genes
b) The inheritance of cognitive and non cognitive aspects of personality
- (1) experimental studies
- (a) rats and selective breeding
- i) maze bright -- maze dull
- ii) reactive, non reactive
- (b) dog breeding
- (2) "simple" genetic models --Mendelian analysis
- (a) single gene models
- i) multiple alleles
- ii) additive genetic variation
- iii) non-additive (dominant/recessive) variation
- (b) multiple gene models
- (3) polygenic inheritance
- (a) simply the sum of separate genes
- i) biometric analysis-- classic Mendelian analysis
- ii) most likely for complex behavioral measures
- (b) analysis of genetic fragments
- (4) the concept of heritability
- (a) Sources of variance
- i) Vp = phenotypic variance in population
- ii) Vg = additive genetic variance
- iii) Vam = variance due to assortative mating
- iv) Vd dominance deviation variance
- v) Vi epistasis (interaction among genes)
- vi) Ve environmental variation
- (1) within family environmental effects
- (2) between family environmental effects
- vii) COVhe covariance of environment and genes
- viii) Veg environment and genetic interaction
- ix) Verror unreliability of measurement
- (b) Common misconceptions about heritability
- i) heredity versus environment -- a meaningless question
- (1) genes code proteins, not behavior
- (2) genes act through the environment
- ii) individual versus population
- (1) variance estimates are population estimates, not for individual
- (2) variations in environment affect estimates
- iii) constancy
- (1) heritability changes by changing any source of variance (h2=Vg/Vp)
- (2) reducing environmental effects increases heritability
- (3) heritability can increase with age
- iv) measurement versus reality
- v) Know all versus know nothing
- vi) immutability
- (1) within current environments
- (2) by changing the environment
- vii) genes act at birth
- (1) some genes have late onset
- (c) empirical findings
- i) consistency across studies and across nations
- ii) for intelligence measures
- (1) Rmza = .75
- (2) Rmzt = .87
- iii) for non-cognitive measures
- (1) h2 ‰ .5±.2
- (2) note that reliabilities are .7-.8
c) Designs
- (1) Mono-zygotic twins reared apart
- (a) Shields' study
- (b) Bouchard/Minnesota Twin Study
- (c) Swedish Twin study
- (2) MZ versus DZ twin designs
- (a) assumptions of equal environments
- (b) basic findings
- i) cognitive measures
- ii) non-cognitive
- (1) extraversion
- (2) neuroticism
- iii) psychopathology
- (1) schizophrenia
- (2) depression
- iv) attitudes
- (3) adoption studies
- (a) the example of schizophrenia
- (b) decreasing similarity of adopteds reared together
d) Conclusions
- (1) moderate additive genetic effect
- (2) moderate environmental effect
- (a) but no between family environmental effect
e) implications
- (1) for personality theory
- (2) for clinical change
- (3) for society